Hearing Voices and Dissociation
Auditory hallucinations: Psychotic symptom or dissociative experience?
“Once you hear the voices, you realize they’ve always been there. It’s just a matter of being tuned to them.”
(Mark Vonnegut, The Eden Express, 1975)
Abstract
While auditory hallucinations are considered a core psychotic symptom, central to the diagnosis of schizophrenia, it has long been recognized that persons who are not psychotic may also hear voices. There is an entrenched clinical belief that distinctions can be made between these groups, typically on the basis of the perceived location or the ‘third-person’ perspective of the voices. While it is generally believed that such characteristics of voices have significant clinical implications, and are important in the differential diagnosis between dissociative and psychotic disorders, there is no research evidence in support of this. Voices heard by persons diagnosed schizophrenic appear to be indistinguishable, on the basis of their experienced characteristics, from voices heard by persons with dissociative disorders or with no mental disorder at all. On this and other bases outlined below, we argue that hearing voices should be considered a dissociative experience, which under some conditions may have pathological consequences. In other words, we believe that, while voices may occur in the context of a psychotic disorder, they should not be considered a psychotic symptom.
Mark Vonnegut’s simple insight, expressed in his compelling autobiographical account of becoming psychotic in America in the 1960s, supports the position put forth in this paper, namely, that hearing voices should be considered a dissociative experience and not a psychotic symptom. This position is opposed to that of most clinicians and researchers, who believe that auditory hallucinations [1] (AH) are best considered a psychotic symptom, or that some AH should be considered psychotic and others dissociative. Nonetheless, our position is supported by both clinical and research evidence, including the evidence that many persons who hear voices do not show any (other) symptom of a mental disorder, and that no experienced characteristic of voices adequately distinguishes between those persons and others who are designated mentally ill. Accordingly, we propose that AH should, under no circumstances, be considered a psychotic symptom, despite the fact that they sometimes occur in the context of a psychotic disorder.
Throughout recorded history, AH have been understood in a number of ways. At various times, and under various circumstances, voices have been viewed as religious or spiritual phenomena (voices of gods, demons, angels), supernatural or psychic experiences (indicative of ghosts or telepathy), psychological experiences (post-traumatic, dissociative, psychotic), or entirely normal (voice of conscience, one’s own thoughts, creative inspiration, grief experiences, hypnopompic and hypnagogic experiences). Over the past 150 years, considerable effort has been expended in attempting to distinguish between these allegedly different forms of hallucinations, particularly along the lines of determining which voices merit the attention of physicians or mental health professionals and which do not.
Typically, these attempts have been mounted around certain perceived characteristics of auditory hallucinations, most commonly whether the voices appear to be emanating from outside the voice hearer or from inside their head (or some other part of their body). Classically, the latter, particularly when combined with ‘intact’ insight, have been referred to as ‘pseudo-hallucinations’, while only the former, i.e., ‘external’ hallucinations, have been accorded the label ‘proper hallucinations’ or ‘true hallucinations’ and considered a psychotic symptom (Jaspers, 1963/1913). However, insight is technically not an experienced aspect of the hallucination but a subsequent evaluation by the voice hearer (essentially equivalent to a secondary delusion). While it appears likely that there are some – as yet unidentified – personality or psychological factors which determine whether voices are heard internally or externally (and help to explain why one person can hear both internal and external voices and why some voices’ perceived source of location may change), there is no evidence that perceived location (or any other experienced characteristic of AH) map onto diagnostic categories or relevant clinical variables (such as treatment response).
Indeed, a recent study pointedly titled ‘On the non-significance of internal versus external auditory hallucinations’ examined this issue in a large cohort and concluded, “(T)he clinical relevance of location is not confirmed, and the conceptual clarity and clinical utility of the pseudohallucination is undermined” (Copolov, Trauer & Mackinnon, 2004, p. 5). Berrios and Dening (1996), in an exhaustive historical review, likened the concept of ‘pseudohallucinations’ to a “‘joker’ in the diagnostic game” whose fluidity allowed clinicians to “call into question the genuineness of some true hallucinatory experiences that do not fit into a pre-conceived psychiatric diagnosis” (p. 761).
Historical overview
Why some people do and others do not hear voices, and how to understand those voices, has been the subject of debate throughout the centuries. The two questions are closely related, as assumptions about voices have usually (but not always) been made by persons who do not hear voices, and their judgments have often been strongly colored by class, gender, and race beliefs. But was there a time when hearing voices was considered entirely normal? While the research data reviewed below suggests, currently, as many as 8-10% of the general population hear voices at some point in their life, one theorist believes that, millennia ago, all people heard voices. Julian Jaynes (1976), in a popular and controversial book, has argued that, until a few thousand years ago, humans did not possess self-reflective consciousness, and heard voices they attributed to gods, which guided their decision-making in small and large matters. He called this ancient mental structure the ‘bicameral’ mind, and argued that people who hear voices in modern times experience a relapse to this form of mind, primarily under the influence of extreme stress. Jaynes also predicted that hearing voices would be associated with activity in the right temporal lobe, analogous to the left hemisphere speech recognition/understanding center known as Wernicke’s area. While Jayne’s highly speculative theory has received little attention in psychiatric circles, there is now some evidence, reviewed below, in support of his contentions.
For much of recorded human history, hearing voices was associated with divine inspiration, or Satanic possession; as such, those who decided the source of others’ voices (often on nebulous criteria) were typically associated with the Christian Church or other religions (Sarbin & Juhasz, 1967). Then, in the mid 16th century, Teresa of Avila, fearful that her nuns would be persecuted by the Inquisition for the voices they heard, argued that some voices should not be seen as divine or devilish, but as the result of more mundane physical problems. She wrote that some voices were due to melancholy, a ‘weak’ imagination, or drowsiness, sleep or sleep-like states (Sarbin & Juhasz, 1967). Importantly, Teresa wrote that such individuals should be treated ‘as if’ they were sick, but Sarbin and Juhasz note that the ‘as if’ was quickly dropped as the phenomenon of hearing voices became medicalized.
The medical perspective became the dominant mode of understanding hallucinations (in all sensory modalities) in the 19th century, a term given its modern definition by Esquirol in 1832. Esquirol distinguished hallucinations from illusions on the basis that the latter involved genuine sensory perceptions (plus distortions); he stated that a person hallucinating “ascribes a body and an actuality to images that the memory recalls without intervention of the senses” (Esquirol, 1832, cited in Bentall, 1990, p. 82). While Esquirol believed that hallucinations were invariably pathological, a dispute developed in subsequent decades as to whether this was so.
This question culminated in a series of debates at the Société Médico-Psychologique in Paris, foreshadowing many of the debates still held today. As summarized by Berrios & Dening (1996), these discussions considered “whether all hallucinations were abnormal; and whether location (internal versus external) and insight (present versus absent) were relevant factors to the definition of pathological hallucinations” (p. 756). Berrios & Dening (1996) note that these important debates were overlooked by Karl Jaspers, in his highly influential General Psychopathology (1963/1913), who attributed the distinctions to an important figure from the 1880s, Victor Kandinsky.
Kandinsky was important not only because, unlike the French theorists, he himself experienced hallucinations, but also because he claimed to experience both ‘true’ (TH) and ‘pseudo’ (PH) hallucinations, and endeavored to distinguish between the two[3]. Kandinsky argued that only TH involved the activity of a subcortical perception center (just like genuine perceptions), while PH involved only the activity of a center for apperception (a term rarely used now, which means the state of being conscious of perceiving) along with a ‘center for abstract, unconscious images’ (Berrios & Dening, 1996). Thus, Kandinsky decided that PH were as sensorially vivid as TH, but lacked the ‘external objectivity’ of TH because in only the latter was the subcortical perception center activated. He also believed
that PH were internally localized.
Auditory hallucinations and differential diagnosis
Auditory hallucinations were not central features of either Kraepelin’s Dementia Praecox or Bleuler’s Schizophrenia. For Bleuler, AH, while occurring frequently in schizophrenia, were derivative of the central disturbance of loosening of associations, and were also common in other disorders. A much closer link between AH and schizophrenia, however, was forged by Kurt Schneider (1959), whose position underpins the diagnosis of schizophrenia in the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) and ICD-10 (WHO, 1993). Schneider felt that certain AH, particularly those commenting on an individual’s thoughts or behaviors or two or more voices conversing with each other, were pathognomic (only one symptom required for a diagnosis) for schizophrenia; he also thought that hearing one’s thoughts aloud was characteristic of the disorder. The first two symptoms were adopted by the American Psychiatric Association for the diagnosis of schizophrenia, and now form two of the three pathognomic symptoms (the third, also problematic, is ‘bizarre’ delusions[4]). Interestingly, while the APA had specifically linked external AH to schizophrenia in early DSMs, this link was eliminated (presumably for lack of empirical evidence) in the DSM-IV (APA, 1994).
There is, however, considerable evidence that AH, particularly those considered pathognomic for schizophrenia, are not only not unique to that disorder but occur more frequently in dissociative identity disorder (Honig et al, 1998; Kluft, 1987; Ross et al, 1989; Ross et al, 1990). In light of this observation, several authors have argued for new dissociative diagnostic categories (‘Dissociative hallucinosis,’ Nurcombe et al, 1996; ‘Dissociative subtype of schizophrenia,’ Ross, 2004) in which AH are featured prominently.
Other authors have taken a different tack, addressing AH directly by proposing new classificatory systems. Thus, Van der Zwaard & Polak (2001), after reviewing the concept of pseudohallucinations, argue that PH should be broken up into several categories, namely:
- nonpsychotic hallucinations (e.g., ‘isolated’ nonpsychotic hallucinations, such as occur in grief reactions, AH arising from sensory deprivation, and ‘vivid internal imagery’, typical in dissociative disorders),
- partial hallucinations, such as ‘fading hallucinations with increasing insight’
- transient hallucinations (such as ‘short lapses’ in reality testing in persons diagnosed with borderline personality disorder).
This classification appears to offer little improvement over the simple PH/TH dichotomy; it seems difficult to justify calling AH in dissociative disorders ‘imagery’ and ‘partial hallucinations’ appears to be a nonsensical concept. Further, there is evidence that AH in borderline personality disorder are not transient, as has been generally believed, but in fact are ‘ongoing and pervasive’ (Yee et al, 2005). In light of this, Yee et al (2005) has offered an alternative classification, namely that AH be split up into:
- normative,
- traumatic-intrusive,
- psychotic,
- organic hallucinations.
But is there any evidence that AH can be successfully carved up in this (or any other) manner?
Research evidence
A number of relevant research studies have now been conducted, examining the prevalence of hallucinations in the general population, contrasting voices heard by patients with non-patients or, within clinical populations, assessing whether certain characteristics of voices predict clinical or outcome variables. Some studies have also demonstrated clear links between AH and dissociative experiences.
AH in non-psychiatric patients
The studies assessing the prevalence and nature of auditory hallucinations in non-psychiatric populations are illustrated in Table 2.
Six large-scale studies, based primarily in the UK, the Netherlands, and the U.S., have found rates of between 0.6% to 8.2% annual or lifetime prevalence of hallucinations in the general population (excluding, to a greater or lesser degree, identified psychiatric patients)[5]. Three studies of selected non-psychiatric samples, primarily university students or medical patients, have reported slightly higher rates, ranging from 2%-13%.
The first large-scale study to assess hallucinations in the general population was carried out primarily in England between 1889 and 1892, and involved interviews with 17,000 adults (Sidgewick, Johnson, Myers, et al., 1894). This study, known as the Sidgewick study after the first author, was conducted by the Society for Psychical Research (SPR), an organization dedicated to researching psychic phenomena, such as telepathy. As such, a number of the interviewers may have been biased toward discovering hallucinations in non-psychiatric patients. However, the study has been described as well designed, and incorporated a number of relevant exclusion criteria (Tien, 1991). Sidgewick et al found approximately 10% of their participants to have experienced visual, auditory, or tactile hallucinations over their lifetime, just under 7% when corrected for sleep-related experiences. Most of those hallucinations were visual. Those experiencing auditory hallucinations decreased from 2% of those in their 20s, to just under 1% for those over 30. In addition, a gender bias was recorded, with women experiencing more hallucinations than men.
Conclusion
In conclusion, the evidence supports our view that AH: a) are best conceptualized as dissociative experiences which appear in individuals predisposed, for reasons not yet clear, to hear voices when under stress, b) require ‘cultivation’ or ‘nurturing’ to make their meaning clear, c) resolve when appropriately engaged with by the individual, possibly moving from externally- to internally-perceived in the process, and d) are consistent with Jaynes’ evolutionary theory of the bicameral mind. Accordingly, we call on our mental health colleagues to no longer designate AH as a psychotic symptom, to conduct research on the remaining areas of confusion in AH (such as determining those factors influencing engagement with voices), and to explore alternative ways of working with the voices heard by many of us.
References
-
- Aggernaes, A. (1972). The experienced reality of hallucinations and other psychological phenomena. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 45, 220-238.
- Allen, J. G., & Coyne, L. (1995). Dissociation and vulnerability to psychotic experience: The Dissociative Experiences Scale and the MMPI-2. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 183(10), 615-622.
- Altman, H., Collins, M., & Mundy, P. (1997). Subclinical hallucinations and delusions in nonpsychotic adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 38(4), 413-420.
- American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
- Barrett, T. R., & Etheridge, J. B. (1992). Verbal Hallucinations in Normals. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 6(5), 379-387.
- Benjamin, L. (1989). Is chronicity a function of the relationship between the person and the auditory hallucination? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 15, 291-310.
- Bentall, R. P. (1990). The illusion of reality: a review and integration of psychological research on hallucinations. Psychological Bulletin, 107(1), 82-95.
- Berrios, G. E., & Dening, T. R. (1996). Pseudohallucinations: A conceptual history. Psychological Medicine, 26(4), 753-763.
- Brewin, C. R. (2003). Posttraumatic stress disorder: Malady or myth. New Haven, Connecticut: Yale University Press.
- Carlson, E. B., & Putnam, F. W. (1986). Development, reliability, and validity of a dissociation scale. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 174, 727-733.
- Chadwick, P., & Birchwood, M. (1994). The omnipotence of voices: A cognitive approach to auditory hallucinations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 190-201.
- Chadwick, P., Birchwood, M., & Trower, P. (1996). Cognitive therapy for delusions, voices, and paranoia. Chichester, U.K.: John Wiley and Sons.
- Close, H., & Garety, P. (1998). Cognitive assessment of voices: Further developments in understanding the emotional impact of voices. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37, 173-188.
- Copolov, D. L., Trauer, T., & Mackinnon, A. (2004). On the non-significance of internal versus external auditory hallucinations. Schizophrenia Research, 69, 1-6.
- David, A. S. (1999). Auditory hallucinations: phenomenology, neuropsychology and neuroimaging update. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 99, 95-104.
- Dening, T. R., & Berrios, G. E. (1996). The enigma of pseudohallucinations: Current meanings and usage. Psychopathology, 29, 27-34.
- Derogatis, L. R. (1994). Symptom Checklist-90-R (SCL-90-R) administration, scoring, and procedures manual (3rd ed.). Minneapolis: National Computer Systems.
- Dhossche, D., Ferdinand, R., van der Ende, J., Hofstra, M. B., & Verhulst, F. (2002). Diagnostic outcome of self-reported hallucinations in a community sample of adolescents. Psychological Medicine, 32(4), 619-627.
- Ensink, B. J. (1992). Confusing realities: A study on child sexual abuse and psychiatric symptoms. Amsterdam: VU Press.
- Escher, S., Romme, M., Buiks, A., Delespaul, P., & van Os, J. (2002a). Independent course of childhood auditory hallucinations: A sequential 3-year follow up study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 181 (suppl. 43), s10-s18.
- Escher, S., Romme, M., Buiks, A., Delespaul, P., & van Os, J. (2002b). Formation of delusional ideation in adolescents hearing voices: A prospective study. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 114, 913-920.
- Escher, A., Morris, M., Buiks, A., Delespaul, P., van Os, J., & Romme, M. (2004). Determinants of outcome in the pathways through care for children hearing voices. International Journal of Social Welfare, 13, 208-222.
- Esquirol, J. E. D. (1832). Sur les illusions des sens chez alienés [On the sensory illusions of the insane]. Archives Generales de Medicine, 2, 5-23.
- Frame, L., & Morrison, A. P. (2001). Causes of posttraumatic stress disorder in psychotic patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 305-306.
- Glicksohn, J. (2004). Absorption, hallucinations, and the continuum hypothesis. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 27(6), 793-794.
- Glicksohn, J., & Barrett, T. R. (2003). Absorption and hallucinatory experience. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17(7), 833-849.
- Hare, E. H. (1973). A short note on pseudo-hallucinations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 122, 469-476.
- Hathaway, S., & McKinley, J. (1967). Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory manual revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation.
- Havens, L. (1962). The placement and movement of hallucinations in space: Phenomenology and theory. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 43(426-435).
- Heins, T., Gray, A., & Tennant, M. (1990). Persisting hallucinations following childhood sexual abuse. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 24, 561-565.
- Hoffman, R. E., Oates, E., Hafner, J., Hustig, H. H., & McGlashan, T. H. (1994). Semantic organization of hallucinated ‘voices’ in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 1229-1230.
- Honig, A., Romme, M. A. J., Ensink, B. J., Escher, S. D. M. A. C., Pennings, M. H. A., & deVries, M. W. (1998). Auditory hallucinations: A comparison between patients and nonpatients. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 186(10), 646-651.
- Jaspers, K. (1963). General Psychopathology (J. Hoenig & M. W. Hamilton, Trans.). Manchester, UK: Manchester University Press. (Original publication date 1913).
- Jaynes, J. (1976). The Origin of Consciousness in the Breakdown of the Bicameral Mind. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
- Johns, L. C., Nazroo, J. Y., Bebbington, P., & Kuipers, E. (2002). Occurrence of hallucinatory experiences in a community sample and ethnic variations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 174-178.
- Kapur, S. (2003). Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: A framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry, 160(1), 13-23.
- Kilcommons, A. M., & Morrison, A. P. (2005). Relationships between trauma and psychosis: an exploration of cognitive and dissociative factors. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 112, 351-359.
- Kingdon, D. G., & Turkington, D. (2005). Cognitive therapy of schizophrenia. New York: Guilford Press.
- Kluft, R. P. (1987). First-rank symptoms as a diagnostic clue to multiple personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 293-298.
- Merckelbach, H., Rassin, E., & Muris, P. (2000). Dissociation, schizotypy, and fantasy proneness in undergraduate students. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 188(7), 428-431.
- Meyer, H., Taiminen, T., Vuori, T., Aijala, A., & Helenius, H. (1999). Posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms related to psychosis and acute involuntary hospitalization in schizophrenic and delusional patients. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 187(6), 343-352.
- Miller, L. J., O’Connor, E., & Di Pasquale, T. (1993). Patients’ attititudes toward hallucinations. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 584-588.
- Morrison, A. P. (1998). A cognitive analysis of the maintenance of auditory hallucinations: Are voices to schizophrenia what bodily sensations are to panic? Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 26, 289-302.
- Morrison, A. P., & Peterson, T. (in press). Trauma, metacognition and predisposition to hallucinations in non-patients. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy.
- Moskowitz, A., Barker-Collo, S., & Ellson, L. (2004). Replication of dissociation-psychoticism link in New Zealand students and inmates. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 193, 722-727.
- Moskowitz, A. K., Read, J., Farrelly, S., Rudegeair, T., & Williams, O. (submitted for publication). Are psychotic symptoms traumatic in origin and dissociative in kind? In P. F. Dell & J. O’Neil (Eds.), Dissociation and the Dissociative Disorders: DSM-V and Beyond.
- Mott, R. H., Small, I. F., & Anderson, J. M. (1965). A comparative study of hallucinations. Archives of General Psychiatry, 12(6), 595-601.
- Nayani, T. H., & David, A. S. (1996). The auditory hallucination: a phenomenological survey. Psychological Medicine, 26(1), 177-189.
- Nurcombe, B., Mitchell, W., Begtrup, R., Tramontana, M., LaBarbera, J., & Pruitt, J. (1996). Dissociative hallucinosis and allied conditions. In F. R. Volkmar (Ed.), Psychoses and pervasive developmental disorders in childhood and adolescence. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
- Ohayon, M. M. (2000). Prevalence of hallucinations and their pathological associations in the general population. Psychiatry Research, 97, 153-164.
- Oulis, P. G., Mavreas, V. G., Mamounas, J. M., & Stefanis, C. N. (1995). Clinical characteristics of auditory hallucinations. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 92(2), 97-102.
- Peralta, V., & Cuesta, M. J. (1999). Diagnostic significance of Schneider’s first-rank symptoms in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 174, 243-248.
- Posey, T. B., & Losch, M. E. (1983-1984). Auditory hallucinations of hearing voices in 375 normal subjects. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 3(2), 99-113.
- Putnam, F. W. (1989). Diagnosis and treatment of Multiple Personality Disorder. New York: Guilford Pres.
- Read, J., & Argyle, N. (1999). Hallucinations, delusions, and thought disorder among adult psychiatric inpatients with a history of child abuse. Psychiatric Services, 50(11), 1467-1472.
- Romme, M. A. J., & Escher, D. M. A. C. (1989). Hearing voices. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 15, 40-47.
- Romme, M. A. J., Honig, A., Noorthoorn, E. O., & Escher, A. D. M. A. C. (1992). Coping with hearing voices: An emancipatory approach. British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 99-103.
- Ross, C. A. (2004). Schizophrenia: Innovations in diagnosis and treatment. New York: Haworth Press.
- Ross, C. A., Anderson, G., & Clark, P. (1994). Childhood abuse and the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Hospital & Community Psychiatry, 45(5), 489-491.
- Ross, C. A., Heber, S., Norton, G. R., & Anderson, G. (1989). Differences between multiple personality disorder and other diagnostic groups on structured interview. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 177(8), 487-491.
- Ross, C. A., Miller, S. D., Reagor, P., Bjornson, L., Fraser, G. A., & Anderson, G. (1990). Schneiderian symptoms in multiple personality disorder and schizophrenia. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 31(2), 111-118.
- Sarbin, T. R., & Juhasz, J. B. (1967). The historical background of the concept of hallucination. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 5, 339-358.
- Sedman, G. (1966a). A comparative study of pseudohallucinations, imagery and true hallucinations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 112(482), 9-17.
- Sedman, G. (1966b). “Inner voices.” Phenomenological and clinical aspects. British Journal of Psychiatry, 112, 485-490.
- Sedman, G. (1966c). A phenomenological study of pseudohallucinations and related experiences. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 42(1), 35-70.
- Schneider, K. (1959). Clinical Psychopathology (5th ed.). New York: Grune and Stratton.
- Sidgewick, H., Johnson, A., Myers, F. W. H., et al. (1894). Report on the census of hallucinations. Proceedings of the Society for Psychical Research, 26, 259-394.
Spitzer, M. (1990). On defining delusions. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 31(5), 377-397.
- Steinberg, M. (1995). Handbook for the assessment of dissociation: A clinical guide. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
- Stone, H., & Winkelman, S. (1989). Embracing our selves: The Voice Dialogue manual. Novato, California: Nataraj Publishing.
- Tandon, R., & Greden, J. F. (1987). Schneiderian first rank symptoms: Reconfirmation of high specificity for schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 75, 392-396.
- Taylor, F. K. (1981). On pseudo-hallucinations. Psychological Medicine, 11, 265-271.
- Tien, A. Y. (1991). Distributions of hallucinations in the population. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 26, 287-292.
- Van der Zwaard, R., & Polak, M. A. (2001). Pseudohallucinations: a pseudoconcept? A review of the validity of the concept, related to associate symptomatology. Comprehensive Psychiatry., 42(1), 42-50.
- Van Os, J., Hanssen, M., Bijl, R. V., & Ravelli, A. (2000). Strauss (1969) revisited: A psychosis continuum in the general population?Schizophrenia Research, 45, 11-20.
- Vonnegut, M. (1975). The Eden Express. New York: Praeger.
- Yee, L., Korner, A. K., McSwiggan, S., Meares, R. A., & Stevenson, J. (2005). Persistant hallucinosis in borderline personality disorder.Comprehensive Psychiatry, 46, 147-154.
- Watson, D. (2001). Dissociations of the night: Individual differences in sleep-related experiences and their relation to dissociation and schizotypy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(4).
- Wiles, N., Sammit, S., Bebbington, P., Singleton, N., Meltzer, H., & Lewis, G. (2006). Self-reported psychotic symptoms in the general population. British Journal of Psychiatry, 188, 519-526.
- World Health Organization. (1993). The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders. Diagnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva: WHO.
Footnotes
- [1] Technically, the term auditory hallucinations refers to any sound heard in the absence of appropriate external stimuli, not only voices. For example, during post-traumatic flashbacks, a wide range of sounds related to a trauma, such as police sirens, gunshots, etc., may be heard. However, for the purposes of this article, auditory hallucinations will be used exclusively to refer to the hearing of voices. While voices are sometimes referred to as ‘verbal’ hallucinations, the term auditory hallucinations is more familiar and as such is preferred here.
- [2] We do not take a position in this paper as to whether AH may, under some circumstances, represent a ‘genuine’ spiritual experience. Even if that is allowed, however, adequate means to distinguish such experiences from AH better explained by psychological (dissociative) mechanisms remains to be established. In other words, at least some AH interpreted by the voice hearer as spiritual may actually derive from a disowned aspect of themselves. At this point, we cannot adequately distinguish between the two on the basis of reported experience.
- [3] The term ‘pseudo-hallucination’ was coined in 1868 by Hagen to refer to hallucinations with less sensory ‘fullness’ than perceptions (or ‘true’ hallucinations), which derived from an ‘organ of apperception’, and which were due to the spontaneous activity of memory (Berrios & Denning, 1996). Almost all theorists have considered ‘pseudo-hallucinations’ to be essentially normal phenomena.
- [4] Examples given of bizarre delusions are beliefs of loss of control of mind or body, and include delusions of control, and delusions of thought insertion and thought withdrawal (APA, 1994). Both of these sets of delusions, along with voices commenting and conversing, also delimit three of the four pathognomic symptoms of ICD-10 Schizophrenia (WHO, 1993). Some question whether these should be called delusions at all, as opposed to adequate descriptions of unusual/anomalous experiences (Spitzer, 1990). All are common in DID, where they are typically understood as deriving from the influence of one part of the personality on another (Kluft, 1987; Ross et al., 1989; Ross et al., 1990).
- [5] A seventh, Wiles et al (2006), is not reported here because incidence and not prevalence data were used. Of interest, however, is that just over 5% of almost 2000 persons, without a psychiatric history and not meeting criteria for a psychotic disorder, reported at least one of four narrowly defined psychotic symptoms (thought insertion, paranoia, ‘strange experiences’ and hallucinations; Wiles et al, 2006).
- [6] Oulis et al (1995) reported that patients had little difficulty in identifying the location from which their voice(s) appeared to emanate, a finding confirmed by most other researchers.
- [7] As Karen also heard two or more voices conversing with each other, her voices fulfilled both of the pathognomic AH criteria for DSM-IV schizophrenia (i.e., voices commenting and voices conversing).
- [8] Indeed, Morrison’s (1998) model of how ‘normal’ AH lead to psychopathology, in which he argues that AH may be ‘misinterpreted’ as “threatening the physical or psychological integrity of the individual,” leading to decreased mood, hyperarousal and hypervigilance, along with avoidance behaviors (p. 296), almost exactly mirrors current conceptions of PTSD.